Behavioral and Visual Ecology in the 21st Century

Light is perhaps the most important abiotic factor driving biological functions. I study how animals have adapted to and are affected by light. How have different light environments selected for different traits such as coloration and vision? How and why have different visual systems evolved? How does anthropogenic light at night affect organisms? I approach these questions from a sensory and behavioral ecological perspective to shed light onto evolutionary and conservation biology.

This page will edited substantially during 2022, please stay tuned.

Visual Ecology: Evolution, Behavior, and Conservation

Overall, my research interests lie at how light affects animals. In most cases, I research the visual perception of light by animals but have studied how light can affect thermoregulatory behaviors as well. Currently I have two main interests within the realm of sensory ecology: 1) Visual physiology and behavioral ecology of anthropogenic light at night; 2) Behavioral and visual ecology of animals. 

Light levels vary over 9 orders of magnitude (a billion times!) from starlight conditions at night to bright sunlight in open habitats. During twilight, these light levels change drastically and this sudden change of light levels is a very important cue for many organisms. Furthermore, natural light levels at night can vary across 4 orders of magnitude depending on lunar phase and lunar altitude. Sidebar - it is tricky to measure such extreme light levels and that is one component of my lab’s research: how does one accurately, quickly (and hopefully without spending 100,000 USD) measure starlight when so much of radiometric equipment is design for bright conditions such as daylight or electric lighting?

Light levels vary over 9 orders of magnitude (a billion times!) from starlight conditions at night to bright sunlight in open habitats. During twilight, these light levels change drastically and this sudden change of light levels is a very important cue for many organisms. Furthermore, natural light levels at night can vary across 4 orders of magnitude depending on lunar phase and lunar altitude. Sidebar - it is tricky to measure such extreme light levels and that is one component of my lab’s research: how does one accurately, quickly (and hopefully without spending 100,000 USD) measure starlight when so much of radiometric equipment is design for bright conditions such as daylight or electric lighting?

A conceptual figure of select biological effects on animals from changing light levels.  Each wedge represents a light environment that ranges within an order of magnitude in lux, the numbers at the end of each spoke on the wheel. The right side shows organismal effects from changes in natural light level changes due to twilight and the lunar cycle. The left side shows organismal effects from artificial light level changes due to human lighting. The inner wedges represent the visual scene. For specifics on organisms and resources, see Seymoure et al 2019 preprint in my publications.

A conceptual figure of select biological effects on animals from changing light levels. Each wedge represents a light environment that ranges within an order of magnitude in lux, the numbers at the end of each spoke on the wheel. The right side shows organismal effects from changes in natural light level changes due to twilight and the lunar cycle. The left side shows organismal effects from artificial light level changes due to human lighting. The inner wedges represent the visual scene. For specifics on organisms and resources, see Seymoure et al 2019 preprint in my publications.


Anthropogenic Light and Visual Ecology

As a postdoctoral fellow with the National Park Service Night Skies Division, I have focused my research on how the overwhelming light produced by humans affects visual systems and ecology of animals. Currently, I have several research topics ranging from the effects of light from oil and gas developments on predator and prey use to the perception of light domes by myriad taxa. Currently I have a few publications from this work and am excited to be submitting several large projects this fall, 2018. These projects include a global map of anthropogenic illuminance relative to natural night levels. I am also a lead on a large collaboration on the traits that are most indicative of species vulnerability to anthropogenic light and anthropogenic noise. Furthermore, we have just finished a large predation study on moths to determine how moth predation is affected by different artificial light sources. And lastly, I am working on a methods piece of how to measure anthropogenic lighting with respect to animals instead of humans, which has historically been the approach. Stay tuned as I add more about this research once these manuscripts are accepted.

Behavioral and Visual Ecology of Animals

An organism's environment can be as dynamic as its behavior. Thus animal behavior and the environment can be tightly linked. One underdeveloped topic in behavioral ecology is how the environment has sculpted animal communication. For visual signals, such as warning colors of toxic butterflies or bright ornaments of male birds, the environment can drastically affect the perception of visual signals by an intended receiver. This can happen through different backgrounds (i.e. sand vs. blue sky), different lighting environments (i.e. shady forest vs. open fields), or different perceptual abilities of receivers (dichromatic vs trichromatic color vision). Due to environmental effects, animals should behave in ways to optimize both production and reception of signals. My research focuses on how the environment has driven and affects visual systems, visual signals, and behavior, mostly in arthropods.

Environmental factors affecting signal efficacy

Visual signals can drastically be affected by available light and the background, and although this has been well known for decades, very little research into the effects of light environment on signal efficacy exists. Fortunately, we now have inexpensive techniques that enable accurate quantification of light environment that can be utilized with studying the ecology and behavior of signal-receiver interactions to shed light onto environmental effects on animal communication. Ongoing research in this sub-discipline includes how perceived conspicuousness of different mimicry rings of Heliconius butterflies is affected by the different available light environments in the tropical rainforest. We also have an ongoing project examining how the sunrise affects conspicuousness of a multicomponent warning signal in the pipeline swallowtail. This project further investigates how animals have evolved and behave to increase signal efficacy.

Physiological ecology of vision

Just as visual signals are dependent upon the environment, visual systems of animals have evolved under certain selection pressures in different environments. Sensory drive predicts that individuals will evolve sensory systems that are able to extract as much information from cues (e.g. mate condition, prey toxicity, etc) as possible. My current research investigates how and why insect visual systems differ as a result of light environment and conspecific interactions. We are currently investigating how different tropical light environments (i.e. nocturnal, forest understory, open fields) has selected for different eye morphology and visual sensitivity. 

Evolution and maintenance of mimetic populations

Over one hundred and fifty years ago, Henry Bates discovered “a most powerful proof of the theory of natural selection” by revealing the fascinating adaptation of an edible animal resembling a conspicuous, inedible animal to reduce risk of predation, termed Batesian mimicry. Then 16 years after Bates’ discovery, Fritz Müller devised one of the first mathematical models supporting natural selection. Müller demonstrated that two conspicuous and unpalatable individuals can both gain protection from predation if they converge on the same warning signal (e.g. yellow and black stripes of wasps), this phenomenon was fittingly dubbed Müllerian mimicry. Therefore, although Batesian and Müllerian mimicry both confuse predators, Batesian mimicry is a parasitic relationship between the edible mimic and the inedible model, while Müllerian mimics mutually benefit from the mimetic relationship. Although mimics can be striking lookalikes of their model, paradoxically, many resemble their model imprecisely. Furthermore, the degree of mimicry precision and accuracy varies within the same species, which begs the question: if natural selection is so powerful, then why are so many mimics imprecise? First, what determines if the mimetic resemblance is precise or imprecise? For visual mimicry (i.e. color and pattern) we now have the ability to mathematically model if two color patches or patterns are noticeably different at the level of the photoreceptors in a perceiver’s eye. These models compare the photon catch of each of the different photoreceptors of the perceiver and then calculates if the differences between the patch reflectance spectra are perceivable when neural noise is added. These visual models allow for an objective definition of imprecise mimicry: the ecologically relevant predator is able to discriminate between the coloration of the model and mimic. We are currently investigating different hypotheses explaining imprecise mimicry in many species of butterflies. 

Examples of imprecise mimicry in butterflies. A) An Ecuadorian mimicry ring that includes species in the family Nymphalidae and Papilionidae. B) Just a few examples of the extensive 'tiger pattern' mimicry of the Amazon. C) The common Monarch, Queen…

Examples of imprecise mimicry in butterflies. A) An Ecuadorian mimicry ring that includes species in the family Nymphalidae and Papilionidae. B) Just a few examples of the extensive 'tiger pattern' mimicry of the Amazon. C) The common Monarch, Queen, Viceroy mimicry system of North and Central America. D) A blue mimicry ring of southern United States, focused on the toxic Pipevine Swallowtail.